Unrepresentative Sample

Alias: Biased Sample

Taxonomy: Logical Fallacy > Informal Fallacy > Weak Analogy > Unrepresentative Sample

Subfallacies:

Form:

N% of sample S has characteristic C.
Therefore, N% of population P has characteristic C.1

Example:

The Literary Digest, which began its famous straw poll with the 1916 presidential campaign, mailed out millions of mock ballots for each of its surveys. …

The results that poured in during the months leading up to the [1936 presidential] election showed a landslide victory for Republican Alf Landon. In its final tabulation, the Digest reported that out of the more than two million ballots it had received, the incumbent, Roosevelt, had polled only about 40 percent of the straw votes. …

Within a week it was apparent that both their results and their methods were erroneous. Roosevelt was re-elected by an even greater margin than in 1932. … The Digest's experience conclusively proved that no matter how massive the sample, it will produce unreliable results if the methodology is flawed.

… The mailing lists the editors used were from directories of automobile owners and telephone subscribers…[which] were clearly weighted in favor of the Republicans in 1936. People prosperous enough to own cars have always tended to be somewhat more Republican than those who do not, and this was particularly true in [the] heart of the Depression.

…The sample was massive, but it was biased toward the affluent, and in 1936 many Americans voted along economic lines.2

Exposition:

This is a fallacy involving statistical inferences, which are arguments of the form shown above. For example, suppose that an opaque jar is full of marbles, and you can win a prize by guessing the proportions of colors of the marbles in the jar. Assume, further, that you are allowed to stick your hand into the jar and withdraw one fistful of marbles before making your guess. Suppose that you pull out ten marbles, six of which are black and four of which are white. The set of all marbles in the jar is the population, P, which you are going to guess about, and the ten marbles that you removed is the sample, S. You want to use the information in your sample to guess as closely as possible the proportion of colors in the jar. You might draw the conclusion that 60% of the marbles in the jar are black and 40% are white.

The strength of a statistical inference is determined by the degree to which the sample is representative of the population, that is, how similar in the relevant respects the sample and population are. If you knew in advance that all of the marbles in a jar are the same color, then you could conclude that the sample is perfectly representative of the color of the population—though it might not represent other aspects, such as size3.

There are three ways that a sample can fail to sufficiently represent the population:

  1. The sample is simply too small to represent the population, in which case the argument will commit the subfallacy of Hasty Generalization, see above. This comes about when the population is variable in the characteristic inferred in the conclusion and the sample is too small to represent that variability. For instance, if the jar contains marbles of more than one color then a single marble could not possibly be a representative sample.
  2. The sample is biased in some way as a result of not having been chosen randomly from the population. The Example is a famous case of such bias in a sample. It also illustrates that even a very large sample can be biased; the important thing is representativeness, not size. Small samples can be representative, and even a sample of one is sufficient in some cases.4
  3. Even though a sample is large enough and chosen at random, it can still be unrepresentative of the population. For instance, even if you thoroughly shook the jar before pulling out a fistful of marbles, it's still possible that you might get only white marbles. There is no guarantee that a sample will be representative.

Exposure:


Notes:

  1. Where sample S is a subset of set P, the population.
  2. Michael Wheeler, Lies, Damn Lies, and Statistics: The Manipulation of Public Opinion in America (Liveright, 1976), pp. 67-9.
  3. When a sample perfectly represents a population, statistical inferences are deductive rather than inductive inferences.
  4. For instance, when cooking a pot of pasta, a single piece is representative of the whole pot and can be tested for doneness.